Difference between revisions of "Lexicalization (in neurocognitive linguistics)"

From Glottopedia
Jump to navigation Jump to search
(new page)
 
(improved readability of the article)
 
(7 intermediate revisions by the same user not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
LEXICALIZATION (in Neurocognitive Linguistics)
+
On '''lexicalization''' Lamb notes that even though a word such as happiness can be understood on the basis of the meanings of its constituent morphemes, the frequency with which this combination occurs is such that the lexicon of the typical speaker will contain not just the separate [[Lexeme (in neurocognitive linguistics)|lexemes]] "happy" and "-ness" but also a complex lexeme "happiness". Moreover, for this to be the case it is unnecessary for the meaning of "happiness" to be in any way idiomatic: It is repeated use rather than degree of idiomaticity that determines presence or absence of a higher-level lexical [[Node (in neurocognitive linguistics)|[node]]].  
‘Lexicalization’ in the title of the paper can be clarified by reference
+
 
to Lamb (1999:163–70, 179, 271), together with information about the frequency of occur-
+
Furthermore, the more frequently any part of the linguistic [[Relational_network|network]] (or wider cognitive network) is used, the easier it is to use it again: "The pathways of the brain are like pathways through a meadow or field or jungle--the more they are used the easier they become to use again".  
rence of particular English words. Lamb notes (163) that even though a word such as hap-
+
 
piness can be understood on the basis of the meanings of its constituent morphemes, the
+
In formalizing this phenomenon in [[Relational network theory|Relational Network Grammar]] (rng), [[Line (in relational network theory)|lines]] of different strengths are used (e.g. they are drawn with different thicknesses) and it is assumed that the strengths of the lines corresponding to frequently used items will increase over time.  
frequency with which this combination occurs is such that the lexicon of the typical speaker
+
 
will contain not just the separate lexemes happy and -ness but also a complex lexeme happi-
+
A Google search on the words happy, happiness, full and fullness, carried out on July 5, 2005, revealed that while the word "happiness" was only 7 times less frequent than "happy", "fullness" was 443 times less frequent than "full". If these figures are representative of a typical speaker's receptive and productive experience with the words in question, it seems reasonable to assume that he/she might either have no single node corresponding to the word "fullness" or, at least, that its connections would be rather weak compared with those of "happiness".
ness. Moreover, for this to be the case it is unnecessary for the meaning of happiness to be in any way idiomatic: ‘it is repeated use rather than degree of idiomaticity that determines
+
 
presence or absence of a higher-level lexical [node]’ (165, 271). Furthermore, the more fre-
+
=== Sources ===
quently any part of the linguistic network (or wider cognitive network) is used, the easier it
+
*David C. Bennett. 2009. [http://www.lacus.org/volumes/32/401_bennett_d.pdf "The Evolution of Clitic Systems: A Lexicalization Explanation]. LACUS Forum XXXII.
is to use it again: ‘The pathways of the brain are like pathways through a meadow or field or
+
*[[Sydney M. Lamb|Lamb, Sydney M.]]. 1999. ''[http://www.benjamins.com/cgi-bin/t_bookview.cgi?bookid=CILT%20170 Pathways of the Brain: The Neurocognitive Basis of Language].''  Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
jungle—the more they are used the easier they become to use again’ (179). In formalizing
+
 
this phenomenon in Relational network Grammar (rng), lines of different strengths are
+
[[Category:En]][[Category:DICT]][[Category:Stratificational_Grammar]]
used (e.g. they are drawn with different thicknesses) and it is assumed that the strengths of
 
the lines corresponding to frequently used items will increase over time. A Google search
 
on the words happy, happiness, full and fullness, carried out on July 5, 2005, revealed that
 
while the word happiness was only 7 times less frequent than happy, fullness was 443 times
 
less frequent than full. If these figures are representative of a typical speaker’s receptive and
 
productive experience with the words in question, it seems reasonable to assume that he/
 
she might either have no single node corresponding to the word fullness or, at least, that its
 
connections would be rather weak compared with those of happiness.
 
~LACUS Forum XXXIII
 
THE EVoLUTIon oF CLITIC SySTEMS:
 
A LEXICALIZATIon EXPLAnATIon
 
David C. Bennett
 
SOAS, University of London, 2009.
 

Latest revision as of 20:28, 31 October 2017

On lexicalization Lamb notes that even though a word such as happiness can be understood on the basis of the meanings of its constituent morphemes, the frequency with which this combination occurs is such that the lexicon of the typical speaker will contain not just the separate lexemes "happy" and "-ness" but also a complex lexeme "happiness". Moreover, for this to be the case it is unnecessary for the meaning of "happiness" to be in any way idiomatic: It is repeated use rather than degree of idiomaticity that determines presence or absence of a higher-level lexical [node].

Furthermore, the more frequently any part of the linguistic network (or wider cognitive network) is used, the easier it is to use it again: "The pathways of the brain are like pathways through a meadow or field or jungle--the more they are used the easier they become to use again".

In formalizing this phenomenon in Relational Network Grammar (rng), lines of different strengths are used (e.g. they are drawn with different thicknesses) and it is assumed that the strengths of the lines corresponding to frequently used items will increase over time.

A Google search on the words happy, happiness, full and fullness, carried out on July 5, 2005, revealed that while the word "happiness" was only 7 times less frequent than "happy", "fullness" was 443 times less frequent than "full". If these figures are representative of a typical speaker's receptive and productive experience with the words in question, it seems reasonable to assume that he/she might either have no single node corresponding to the word "fullness" or, at least, that its connections would be rather weak compared with those of "happiness".

Sources